LEARNING THEORIES
LEARNING THEORIES
1. WHAT ARE THE LEARNING THEORIES?
Learning Theory describes how students absorb, process, and retains knowledge
during learning. Cognitive, emotional, and environmental influences, as
well as prior experience, all play a part in how understanding, or a world
view, is acquired or changed and knowledge and skills retained.
Behaviorists
look at learning as an aspect of conditioning and advocate a system
of rewards and targets in education.
Educators
who embrace cognitive theory believe that the definition of learning
as a change in behaviour is too narrow, and study the learner rather than their
environment—and in particular the complexities of human memory. Those who
advocate constructivism believe that a learner's ability to learn
relies largely on what they already know and understand, and the acquisition of
knowledge should be an individually tailored process of
construction. Trans-formative learning theory focuses on the
often-necessary change required in a learner's preconceptions and world view.
Geographical learning theory focuses on the ways that contexts and environments
shape the learning process.
Outside
the realm of educational psychology, techniques to directly observe the
functioning of the brain during the learning process, such
as event-related potential and functional magnetic resonance
imaging, are used in educational neuroscience. The theory of multiple
intelligences, where learning is seen as the interaction between dozens of
different functional areas in the brain each with their own individual
strengths and weaknesses in any particular human learner, has also been
proposed, but empirical research has found the theory to be unsupported by
evidence.
Learning is an enduring change in behaviour, or the capacity to behave in a given fashion which
results from practice or other forms of experience (Chunk, 2012). Learning can
also be looked at as a relative permanent change of behaviour as a result of
experience.
Learning theories are theories whose main concern is to link research with education. In other words
learning theories explain how learning and teaching processes should be and/or
should take place. As teachers deal with teaching and of equal importance
learning of students, the contribution of various learning theories to teacher
development is with some detail given hereunder. Although theories differ in
many ways, including their general assumptions and guiding principles, many
rest on a common foundation. These theories differ in how they predict that
learning occurs—in the processes of learning—and in what aspects of learning
they stress. Thus, some theories are oriented more toward basic learning and
others toward applied learning and, within that, in different content areas;
some stress the role of development, others are strongly linked with
instruction; and some emphasize motivation.
Overview of Learning Theories
Although
there are many different approaches to learning, there are three basic types of
learning theory: behaviorist, cognitive constructivist, and social
constructivist. This section provides a brief introduction to each type of
learning theory. The theories are treated in four parts: a short historical
introduction, a discussion of the view of knowledge presupposed by the theory,
an account of how the theory treats learning and student motivation, and,
finally, an overview of some of the instructional methods promoted by the
theory is presented.
Behaviorism
|
Cognitive Constructivism
|
Social Constructivism
|
|
View of knowledge
|
Knowledge is a repertoire of
behavioral responses to environmental stimuli.
|
Knowledge systems of cognitive structures
are actively constructed by learners based on pre-existing cognitive
structures.
|
Knowledge is constructed within
social contexts through interactions with a knowledge community.
|
View of learning
|
Passive absorption of a predefined
body of knowledge by the learner. Promoted by repetition and positive
reinforcement.
|
Active assimilation and
accommodation of new information to existing cognitive structures. Discovery
by learners is emphasized.
|
Integration of students into a
knowledge community. Collaborative assimilation and accommodation of new
information.
|
View of motivation
|
Extrinsic, involving positive and
negative reinforcement.
|
Intrinsic; learners set their own
goals and motivate themselves to learn.
|
Intrinsic and extrinsic. Learning
goals and motives are determined both by learners and extrinsic rewards
provided by the knowledge community.
|
Implications for Teaching
|
Correct behavioral responses are
transmitted by the teacher and absorbed by the students.
|
The teacher facilitates learning by
providing an environment that promotes discovery and
assimilation/accommodation.
|
Collaborative learning is
facilitated and guided by the teacher. Group work is encouraged.
|
2. EXAMINE GAGNE’S LEARNING THEORY
WITH PARTICULAR FOCUS ON ITS IMPLICATION FOR THE DEVELOPMENT OF SCIENCE
CURRICULUM
Introduction
Robert Gagne's theories and research have had a significant impact on
practitioners in general, especially instructional designers. He has influenced
teaching and curriculum development and used standard practices as a stimulus
for the development of theory. This paper explores Gagne's influence on
practice by examining the relationship between theory and practice, especially
in relation to instructional design, and then discussing science curriculum
development and transfer of learning. Gagne wanted to apply theory to practice,
and was especially interested in applying theory to teaching and learning to
make it more effective and efficient. This paper includes discussions of the
literature on Gagne which covers the practical use of his cumulative learning
theory, his notion of the learning hierarchy in educational curriculum, and
the importance of learner outcomes when analyzing content of instructional
design literature and practice. The influence of Gagne's theories on
instructional design practice spans a gap from a reliance on behaviorism as a
fundational theory to the eventual adoption of cognitivism as an underlying
theory. Gagne's theories and research have been applied to a wide variety of
content areas, age levels, and learning environments.
Robert Gagne's theories and research in instruction and learning have
been discussed in other chapters, where their relationships to each other are
explored in depth. Gagne's theories and research have had significant impact on
practitioners in general and of instructional designers in specific, and this
will be the focus of this paper. Given the length of his professional career,
and the esteem with which his numerous publications are held, it is axiomatic
that he has had an impact. Further examination reveals that he also has
influenced teaching and science curriculum development through his research and
theory. He also used standard practices as a stimulus for the development of
theory. Throughout his career, Gagne was always cognizant of the gap between theory
and practice, and addressed this gap by directing his investigations toward
practical problems
Impact of Gagne's Theories on Science Curriculum Development Practice
An examination of curriculum and curriculum development logically begins
with a concept definition. Gagne (1966) defines curriculum as, a sequence of
content units arranged in such a way that the learning of each unit may be
accomplished as a single act, provided the capabilities described by specified
prior units (in the sequence) have already been mastered by the learner. (p.
22) this orientation is a logical extension of his cumulative learning theory
and his notion of the learning hierarchy. Contrasting definitions illustrate
the diversity of thinking in this area. For example, Bloom (1976) views
curriculum as occurring in two forms visible and invisible. The former being
the school subjects’ one is taught, and the latter being those lessons which
teach one his or her place in the world. Gagne's concept is closer to the first
view. Further contrasting definitions are offered by Bruner (1966)2, Eisner
(1985)3, and Klein (American Society for Curriculum Development, 1993)4. Bruner
and Klein provide views that are more traditional and closer to that of Gagne.
Eisner. on the other hand, also recognizes the existence of both formal and
informal curricula, similar to Bloom. While not all theorists agree on the
definition of curriculum, Gagne's position has been used as the basis for a
number of important efforts in schools and training.
School Program Design
The most pervasive example of an application of Gagne's theories and
research to a large scale curriculum project is Science: A Process Approach
(SAPA), which is part of the American Association for the Advancement of
Science (AAAS) Commission on Science Education. These science curriculum
materials were influential in schools and colleges during the 1960s and early
1970s and represent a significantly large scale curriculum effort utilizing
Gagne's theories and research in the areas of problem solving and scientific
inquiry. Gagne's view of a process approach to science is scientific inquiry
and is based on students having a large knowledge base which they subsequently
utilize to make and then test inductive inferences. The underlying foundation
for the process approach is hierarchical, and presumes that learners have the
prerequisite process skills as background.
Gagne (1965) maintained that the process approach is a middle ground
between the "content approach" and the "creative approach"
and "It substitutes the notion of having children learn generalizable
process skills which are behavioral specific, but which carry the promise of
broad transferability across many subject matters" (p. 4). It can also be
said that SPAP and its orientation to teaching elementary science and
scientific inquiry, although first published in the sixties, remained immensely
influential in science texts and other commercially published science materials
well into the 1980s. Andrew Ahlgren of AAAS, coauthor of Science for All
Americans, provided further testimony to Gagne's influence on science
curriculum, as well as his indirect influence on mathematics, and technology
curriculum in specific (A. Ahlgren, October 3, 1994, personal communication).
He stated that SAPA most certainly had tremendous influence on not only
science, but also technology curriculum. Not all see Gagne's influence on
science curriculum as positive.
Finiley (1983), for example, argues that
Gagne's theories, as well as others of like mind, have propelled science
curriculum in the wrong direction by advocating a commitment to inductive
empiricism.5 He maintains that a presentation of papers by Gagne to AAAS
". . . has had a substantial influence on curriculum, instruction, and
research in science education since that presentation" (p. 47). Finiley
then selects Gagne, in view of all others writing about science process, as the
most influential when he says: "Although many science educators have
written about science processes, the view established by Gagne has been most
influential" (p. 48). He continues his argument from a philosophical
perspective indicating that Gagne, similar to his predecessors like Francis
Bacon, Robert Boyle, Sir Isaac Newton and Hume, embrace the positions of
empiricism and induction. Finiley, although in fundamental disagreement with
Gagne's approach to teaching science, substantiates the overreaching influence
Gagne has had on the development of science through SAPA during the late 1960s
and into the 1980s.
Hackett (1971) provides another example of the use of Gagne's theories on
a large scale curriculum project in a public school setting. Although her work
was primarily directed toward reading and communication skills curricula, she
provides ample evidence of the application of Gagne's theories to social
studies and mathematics as well. Hackett's experiments and curriculum projects focused
on a performance-based approach which has many similarities to the outcome
based education movement of the late 1980s and early 1990s.
There are also many examples of smaller scale curriculum efforts that
apply Gagne's theory to curriculum development projects. Two examples are
Gilbert's (1992) use of Gagne's hierarchies in his curriculum on questioning
and taxonomies, and Lines's (1988) work with advanced economics. These programs
provide evidence of more recent applications of Gagne's theories to curriculum.
One can also examine as evidence Margaret E.Bell's (1982) article in which she
makes a persuasive case for the application of Gagne's theories to designing
programs. She argues that curriculum design and development has not been as
systematic as the efforts of designing instruction. Bell recommends that
Gagne's five capatilities can be applied to course instruction as well as
program or curriculum development. John Flynn's (1992) also adapts Gagne's
Events of Instruction to the very high profile and contemporary research area
of coopezative learning.
School Lesson Design
When relating Gagne's theories to curriculum
efforts that are directed toward individual lessons many of the examples
utilize computer technology. Lesgold's (1987) effort where in goal knowledge
was examined as to its significance to ". intelligent machine [and] human activity.
" is an example of adapting Gagne's theories to curriculum and
prerequisite skills in a novel way. Also in this category is the Smaldino and
Thompson (1990) research relating the Events of Instruction to science
education and computer technology. These authors propose designing science
lessons focusing on the "Nine Events of Instruction" (p. 17).
Jonassen (1988) has utilized many of Gagne's writings, theories and principles
in the design of microcomputer courseware. He especially utilizes Gagnes Events
of Instruction and his work in the area of hierarchies and prerequisite skills.
Jonassen (1988) also utilizes Gagne's work with respect to learning outcomes in
designing individual lessons to be delivered by computer courseware
3. WHAT IS THE RELEVANCE OF PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORY OF LEARNING TO SCIENCE TEACHING?
The
theory depicts to teachers on the role of organizing properly the process of
teaching and learning, so as to make sure that processing of information goes
smoothly
-
The theory also shows that curriculum should be organized in such a way that
the sequence of materials reflects the notion of repetition so that the content
at one level is built on the basis of the previous one.
-
The theory also stipulates the kind of knowledge and the way learners can
inculcate them .These are procedural knowledge and declarative. Where it is
known that procedural knowledge needs more emphasis and time than declarative
knowledge.
THE RELEVANCE PSYCHOLOGICAL THEORIES OF
LEARNING AND PERSPECTIVES CONCERNING THE TEACHING AND LEARNING OF SCIENCE
Active Learning: Learn by Doing
Active learning is a set of strategies that posits the responsibility
for learning with the student. Discovery learning,
problem-based learning (22.3), experiential learning, and inquiry-based
instruction (22.1) are examples of active learning. Discussion, debate (22.4),
student questioning (5.1, 22.1, 23.1), think-pair-share (25.7), quick-writes
(25.7), polling, role playing, cooperative learning (22.3, 22.5), group
projects (13.1-8, 22.5), and student presentations (22.4) are a few of the many
activities that are learner driven. It should be noted, however, that even
lecture can be an active learning event if students processes and filter
information as it is provided. Cornell notes (3.1) and diagramming (16.2)
are a couple of activities that can make lectures active learning events.
Teaching to multiple
learning modalities
We can learn through any of
our five senses, but the three most valuable are vision, hearing, and touch.
Theorists and practitioners claim that learners have a preference for one
learning style over another. Visual learners learn best by watching,
while auditory learners learn best by verbal instruction, and kinesthetic
learners learn best by manipulation. Because of the demands of the
profession, teachers often resort to the instructional style that requires the
least time and preparation, namely lecture and discussion. Although these
may be valuable approaches to teaching and learning, they fail to take
advantage of other learning modalities, and disenfranchise students whose
primary modality is visual or kinesthetic. Throughout this book we
emphasize the use of all three modalities in teaching and learning.
Teaching to multiple intelligence
Intelligence is a property
of the mind that includes many related abilities such as the capacities to
reason, plan, solve problems, comprehend language and ideas, learn new
concepts, and think abstractly. Historically, psychometricians have measured
intelligence with a single score (intelligence quotient, IQ) on a standardized
test, finding that such scores are predictive of later intellectual
achievement. Howard Gardner and others assert
that there are multiple intelligences, and that no single score can accurately
reflect a person’s intelligence. More importantly, the theory of multiple
intelligences implies that people learn better through certain modalities than
others, and that the science teacher should design curriculum to address as
many modalities as possible. Gardner identifies seven intelligences,
which are listed below. The numbers in parentheses indicate sections in
this book that address each intelligence.
- Logical
/Mathematical Intelligence is used when thinking conceptually (6.1-4,
7.1-7, 10.1-5, 13.9, 16.1-6, 18.1-3), computing (14.1-3, 15.1-7, 17.1-7,
20.1, 20.8), looking for patterns (1.1-4,16.4, 16.6, 17.5-7), and
classifying (8.1-6, 19.1-5)
- Linguistic/Language
Intelligence is used when learning by listening (21.1), verbalizing
(1.1-4, 3.1-4, 11.2-4, 22.6), reading (2.1-4), translating (14.1-3), and
discussing (8.6, 22.4).
- Naturalist
Intelligence is used to question (5.1, 22.1, 23.1), observe (5.2-3, 22.2),
investigate (23.2), and experiment (5.1-10, 23.3-4).
- Visual
/ Spatial Intelligence is used when learning with models (12.1-5),
photographs (16.4, 16.6), videos (16.5), diagrams (8.1-6, 16.1-3, 20.2-7),
maps (21.1-7) and charts (20.2-7).
- Bodily
kinesthetic intelligence is used to process knowledge through bodily
sensations (12.2), movements (12.2), physical activity (labs in companion
volumes, Hands-on Chemistry and Hands-on
Physics), and manipulation (22.2).
- Interpersonal
Intelligence is used when learning through cooperative learning
experiences (22.3, 22,5), group games (13.1-8), group lab work (22.5), and
dialog (8.6, 23.4).
- Intrapersonal
Intelligence is used when learning through self-dialog (7.1-3,11.1),
studying (11.2-4) and self-assessment (7.4-7).
- Musical Intelligence is used when
learning through rhythm, melody, and non-verbal sounds in the environment
(24.8).
Metacognition: Teaching
students to think about their thinking
John Flavel argues that learning is maximized when students learn
to think about their thinking and consciously employ strategies to maximize
their reasoning and problem solving capabilities. A metacognitive thinker
knows when and how he learns best, and employs strategies to overcome barriers
to learning. As students learn to regulate and monitor their thought
processes and understanding, they learn to adapt to new learning challenges.
Expert problem solvers first seek to develop an understanding of problems by
thinking in terms of core concepts and major principles (6.1-4, 7.1-7,
11.1-4). By contrast, novice problem solvers have not learned this
metacognitive strategy, and are more likely to approach problems simply by
trying to find the right formulas into which they can insert the right numbers.
A major goal of education is to prepare students to be flexible for new
problems and settings. The ability to transfer concepts from school to the work
or home environment is a hallmark of a metacognitive thinker (6.4).
Developing higher order reasoning
Perhaps the most widely
used classification of human thought is Bloom’s Taxonomy.
Benjamin Bloom and his team or researchers wrote extensively on the
subject, particularly on the six basic levels of cognitive outcomes they
identified – knowledge, comprehension, application, analysis, synthesis, and
evaluation. Bloom’s taxonomy (6.1) is hierarchical, with knowledge,
comprehension and application as fundamental levels, and analysis, synthesis
and evaluation as advanced (6.1-6.4). When educators refer to “higher level
reasoning,” they are generally referring to analysis, synthesis and/or
evaluation. One of the major themes of this book is to develop higher
order thinking skills through the teaching of science.
Constructivism: Helping students build their understanding of science
Constructivism is a major learning theory, and is particularly
applicable to the teaching and learning of science. Piaget suggested
that through accommodation and assimilation, individuals construct new
knowledge from their experiences. Constructivism views learning as a
process in which students actively construct or build new ideas and concepts
based upon prior knowledge and new information. The constructivist
teacher is a facilitator who encourages students to discover principles and
construct knowledge within a given framework or structure. Throughout
this book we emphasize the importance of helping students connect with prior
knowledge and experiences as new information is presented, so they can dispense
with their misconceptions (7.4-7) and build a correct understanding.
Seymour Papert, a student of Piaget, asserted that
learning occurs particularly well when people are engaged in constructing a
product. Papert’s approach, known as constructionism, is facilitated by
model building (12.5), robotics, video editing (16.5), and similar construction
projects.
Pedagogical content knowledge (PCK) in science
An expert scientist is not
necessarily an effective teacher. An expert science teacher, however,
knows the difficulties students face and the misconceptions they develop, and
knows how to tap prior knowledge while presenting new ideas so students can
build new, correct understandings. Schulman refers
to such expertise as pedagogical content knowledge (PCK), and says that
excellent teachers have both expert content knowledge, and expert PCK. In
How People Learn, Bransford, Brown and Cocking state: “Expert teachers have a
firm understanding of their respective disciplines, knowledge of the conceptual
barriers that students face in learning about the discipline, and knowledge of
effective strategies for working with students. Teachers' knowledge of their
disciplines provides a cognitive roadmap to guide their assignments to
students, to gauge student progress, and to support the questions students
ask.” Expert teachers are aware of common
misconceptions and help students resolve them. This book is dedicated to
improving science teacher pedagogical content knowledge.
CONCLUSION
Gagne's
theories provide a great deal of valuable information to teachers. Applying
Gagne's nine-step model is an excellent way to ensure an effective and
systematic learning program as it gives structure to the lesson plans and a
holistic view to the teaching. We need to keep in mind that the exact form of
these events is not something that can be specified in general for all lessons,
but rather must be decided for each learning objective.
Concluding
from the examination of Gagne's influence on science curriculum it is clear
that his work has been significant. Evidence of his influence can be found in
the many applications of his theories and research to a wide variety of content
areas, age levels and learning environments. Additionally, his theories have
withstood the test of time having been applied to curriculum of various types
over the course of 50 plus years. As mentioned earlier his influence on the
curriculum of science has perhaps been most broad based, long lasting and
nationally acclaimed.
A
final comment on Gagne's future influence on curriculum must take into account
the writers, researchers and theorists in curriculum publications. These
documents would lead one to conclude that constructivism will be the dominant
force in curriculum construction in the nineties. Earlier, when discussing
curriculum, it was noted that Gagne was cited only once in the 1991-94 PSCD
Handbook while constructivism and situated cognition were cited often.
Furthermore, the ASCD publications have generous citations, methods, and corresponding
activities that are very situated or constructivist in nature.
REFERENCES
American
Association for the Advancement of Science. (1989). Science for all Americana.
A project 2061 report on literacy goals in science, mathematics, and
technology. Washington, D.C.: Author.
American
Association for the Advancement of Science Commission on Science Education.
(1965). The psychological bases of science A process approach. Washington, D.
C.: AAAS. American Society for Curriculum Development. (1993). Curriculum
handbook: A resource for curriculum administrators from American Society for
Curriculum Development. Alexandria, Virginia: The Educational and Technology
Resource Center ASCD. Anglin, G. (1992). Reference citations in selected
instructional design and technology. Educational Technoloey Research and
Development, 4, 40-43
Aggarwal,
J. C. (2004). Essentials of Educational Psychology: (6th Edition).
Delhi: Vikas Publishing House PVT.
Ary,
D., Jacobs, L. C. and Sorensen, C (2010). Introduction to Research in Education. Belmont,
CA, USA: Wadsworth.
Ashcraft,
M.H. (1994). Human memory and cognition. New York: Harper Collins
Atherton,
J. S., (2011). Learning and Teaching; Piaget's developmental
theory retrieved 19th March 2012
from http://www.learningandteaching.info/learning/piaget.htm
No comments